University of Idaho Psychology of Learning
Lesson 2: Lecture 4 Transcript
 
Home
Syllabus
Schedule
Contact
Help

 

Department of Psychology

  ©
 
University of Idaho
  All rights reserved.

  Psychology Dept.
  University of Idaho

 


 

 


 

 

Back  
Transcript of Audio Lecture
In the last section, we began talking about some aspects of classical conditioning and some variables that were associated with the classical conditioning. In this section, we’re going to begin to determine whether or not, and what factors determine whether a conditioned response will actually develop. We begin this process by discussing the concept of contiguity, which is shown in slide two.

In essence, what contiguity says is this, that the conditioned stimulus must precede the unconditioned stimulus, even if only for a brief time period. In general, the longer the delay between the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus, the poorer the conditioning. We will talk about this in more detail as we talk about this concept in schedules of conditioning.

In essence, what’s happened is that the conditioned stimulus as we see in slide three, must act as a cue, and the conditioned stimulus that provides the most reliable information about the occurrence of an unconditioned stimulus is going to be chosen over all the other stimulus cues that are present in the environment. In essence, the conditioned stimulus must predict the unconditioned stimulus the best. However, other cues could also prevent or block the development of the conditioned response and we will talk about that at a little bit later time.

Now when we’re talking about classical conditioning, as we see in slide four, there’s an optimal period for learning. It also depends on the organism, the conditioned stimulus that we’re using, and the unconditioned stimulus we are using. Let’s give some examples to give us some context.

The classic example begins with looking at an eye blink reflex. What we want to do when we present the conditioned stimulus and the unconditioned stimulus is to have it very rapid. And as you see with the other ones as we go down the list, two seconds for skeletal movements, four seconds for salivary responses and on and on and on. We can see this change and increase in time occurs. The question becomes why? Primarily it is due to the latency of respondents, or responding in the particular reflex system. So, it’s not just the fact that we’re having the relationship together, but that there’s an optimal period of time for the presentation of the CS and the unconditioned stimulus. And as we see in slide five, there is an actual relationship that you can do. This changes for each type of system and learning.

Now there’s one exception to this rule and that is shown in slide six. This is called Poison Based Avoidance Conditioning (also called PBAC). Long, delayed conditioning can also occur (several hours) The question becomes why? What is an example of this particular issue?

Poison based avoidance conditioning, basically occurs when an organism eats some food, a period of time elapses (could be hours), and the organism gets sick. The classic example goes something like this. A long time ago in a galaxy far, far away I went to a restaurant and had a new dish ( of food) that I had never tried before. And after eating that food, I went home. In about seven or eight hours I got astronomically sick (puking, vomiting, etc.) From then on, I have never liked that particular food, and I’ve never had that particular food again. Why, because I developed an association between the particular food and the nausea and vomiting. Now in all likelihood, I probably had some stomach flu or something else. But that hasn’t changed my view of this particular type of food and I still haven’t eaten it since.

A second variable that will determine whether a conditioned response will develop is shown in slide seven. This relates to the strength of the unconditioned stimulus. In general, the stronger the unconditioned stimulus, the faster the learning. And the stronger the unconditioned stimulus, the higher the level of the unconditioned response that occurs.

In addition,as we see in slide eight, the strength of the conditioned stimulus will have a major impact on the conditioned response. Also, the strength of UCS can significantly influence the CR. Sometimes, a conditioned stimulus will cause a conditioned response to increase and sometimes it doesn’t. The question becomes why? When you only get one conditioned stimulus, you may get a weak conditioned response, but if you get a weak and a strong CS, you will get greater conditioned responding to the strong conditioned stimulus. For example, if you are giving two tones, (a weak tone versus a strong tone) the organism will go for the strong tone over the weak tone.

The next variable that will determine whether a conditioned response will occur is shown in slide nine and that’s the saliency or the nature of the conditioned stimulus. Stimuli differ in their abilities to become associated with UCS. That is, some will become conditioned and some will not. Salience, in essence, refers to the degree of associative ability (how things that will cause that particular pairing to occur). In essence, salient stimuli become highly associated and non-salient stimuli do not become associated.

The next variable is, relates to the conditioned stimulus delivery. This is shown in slide 10. That is, the conditioned stimulus must precede the unconditioned stimulus and no conditioning will occur if the UCS occurs without the CS as often as occurs with it (50/50 ratio presentation). In essence, what this means, is that there must be some predictability of the CS with the UCS. No conditioned response will occur if the CS occurs without the UCS. That is, if you ring the bell alone, no conditioned response actually occurs.

The next variable is called predictiveness. This was shown by Bowles. Bowles believed that the contiguity of stimuli was not enough. That is, the events must occur together before we get a conditioned response. However, as we talked about earlier, the CS must predict the UCS and not some other stimuli. So, of all the stimuli that are out there, and all the unconditioned stimuli that are out there, the CS must predict one particular UCS and not some neutral stimulus or conditioning will not occur.

The next concept, as we see in slide 12, relates to blocking. Bowles suggested the cues must first, predict the UCS and provide information not signaled by other cues in the environment. A man named Kamin basically demonstrated that the presence of

positively - predicted cues will prevent or block the development of an association with a second cue. This is shown in an experiment beginning on slide 14. In phase one, Kamin gave rats a distinctive CS (a light), and then he paired it with the UCS (a shock). Then he gave a second phase. He put rats in the two groups. The rats in the experimental group received eight pairings of light with the conditioned stimulus and a second cue (a tone). So, we present a light, then we’re present a tone and then the shock. A control group only received the tone with the shock but no light. The results showed when he presented the light only, the rats didn’t bar-press. When he presented the tone only, the rats continued to bar-press. That is, it had no effect. The light became associated with the shock and the tone did not. That is, the light blocked the tone and so the rats in the experimental group continued to bar press, but the rats in the control group did not continue bar press. The question for this is shown in this next slide, why?

In essence, what happens, says Kamin is that the presence of the light causes the experimental animals to not pay attention to the tone. For surprise, they receive the shock. So in essence, surprise causes the rats to associate the light with the shock. Later on, when you present the light with the shock, the animal was always surprised before the tone was presented. So in essence what we find is when we present CS1, then CS2 , and then the unconditioned stimulus, over a period of time and pairings, CS1 will in essence block CS2.

That is different from higher order conditioning where CS2 is presented first, then CS1, and a conditioned response occurs. In essence CS2 and in higher order conditioning causes a conditioned response. So there’s a difference. In blocking, you have CS1, then CS2, then you get a conditioned response. In higher order conditioning, you get a CS2 then a CS1 and then the conditioned response. I would suggest that you write these down, the differences between these two, and make sure that you understand them for our discussions at a later time.

And so we will close in this section and we’ll begin by talking about some other variables shortly, so until that time, have yourself a great day.


Back