University of Idaho Psychology of Learning
Lesson 5: Lecture 4 Transcript
 
Home
Syllabus
Schedule
Contact
Help

 

Department of Psychology

  ©
 
University of Idaho
  All rights reserved.

  Psychology Dept.
  University of Idaho

 


 

 


 

 

Back  
Transcript of Audio Lecture

In the last section we began an examination of some factors that influenced memory. In this section, we want to begin an examination of some early theories of memory or what are called stage or multiprocess theories. Of these stage or multiprocess theories the best of these is described by Atkinson and Shiffrin. So let’s begin with a discussion of some of these stage or multiprocess theories. This is shown in slide two.

In slide two, there’s a schematic of the Atkinson and Shiffrin model and we’re going to walk through this model. You will need to keep coming back to this slide off and on, so you might print it off so you have an idea about what it is. Let’s look at the first aspect of the sensory system.

The first thing you need to note right off the bat is that you have some kind of a sensation and the sensation can be anything. It can be an auditory sensation, a tactile sensation, a visual sensation, etc., and this sensation moves into what Atkinson and Shiffrin call sensory memory. In sensory memory (as we see in slide three), sensory impressions are stored in a form similar to the original sensation and there’s a wide variety of different sub-groups. Let’s look at the first one of these which is called Iconic memory.

Iconic memory is related to the visual system. Basically these are visual images in the retina and the brain that last approximately a quarter second. They can also last longer, but usually they’re gone within a quarter second. One question (if you’re asking) is why does this image stay within the visual system. That’s because your bipolar, ganglion, amacrine, and horizontal cells in your retina and other cells within your brain primarily in the superior colliculus and occipital lobe are working and processing the different pieces of material.

Now you may ask, “Well what really is an iconic memory, how do I know what it is?” So, what I’d like you to do is find something in the room and start looking at it. The object can be anything. So find something and look at it. Then I want you to close your eyes and note what you immediately see right after you close your eyes.

What you will see is that object. It will last for a little bit of time and then fade away. That is sensory iconic memory.

Now the next type of sensory memory relates to what is called echoic memory. This is shown in slide five. Echoic memory is related to your auditory system. Basically these sounds are in your cochlea and in your temporal lobe. They last two seconds, or less, but sometimes an last longer. Again the question is why. The reason is the sound waves you have in the cochlea are occurring. You have fluid waves occurring there. As a result, you continue to hear things. Now, this duration can last longer than two seconds. A classic example would be if you go out and listen and somebody shoots off a firecracker right behind you. How long does the sound last. Well, the sound can last longer than two seconds. In fact, it can last five to ten seconds. And if the sound is really loud, it can last twenty seconds. So you have two memory systems that are related to what we call sensory memory. There are also other types of sensory memory as well but these two are the most important.

Well let’s go back now and look at slide two again. What we have is information that’s going into our sensory memory. Once this material is in sensory memory, two things can happen. Number one, is you can pay attention to the particular item and if that’s the case, the information moves into the next memory system that is called short term memory which we’ll talk about in a minute. Number two, if you don’t pay attention to the particular item, the particular item is forgotten.

So let’s say that you pay particular attention to some kind of an iconic or echoic memory and it moves into short term memory. Let’s talk about short term memory, what it is, and how it works. We begin in slide six.

Basically short term memory consists of two things; first, events that have just occurred that are still in your consciousness. So what I just said a minute ago might still be in there. Second, different events that are brought back by recall from long term memory. So, short-term memory can consist of things that have just occurred, or information that you brought back from long-term recall.

Now as we see in slide seven, information in short-term memory can do one of three things. This is shown in slide two with a graphic. Number one, it can be rehearsed and remain in short term memory. Or, however, if it is not rehearsed, it is forgotten. So you just lose it. Or, finally it can go enter the next stage of memory (long term memory). So in each case, the memory that you have in short term memory is going to do one of those three things. You can rehearse it and keep it in there, if you don’t rehearse it, the memory goes away, or it can go into long term storage.

Well we have this information within our short term memory. The question became, “What’s the capacity of short term memory?” This is shown in slide eight and relates to seven plus or minus two. The capacity of STM was shown by Miller at Bell Labs. What Miller found is that you recall seven plus or minus two chunks or items that you can retain in short term memory. However, it lasts a very brief/short period of time (ten to twenty seconds). That is one reason (Miller worked for Bell Labs, ala Bell Telephone), why your telephone number is seven items. Or if it’s seven chunks of items, you basically have a one and international code, an area code and then your local prefix and finally your number. So, you can recall your phone number (with a maximum of about twenty to thirty digits of information) because you chunk it out.

The same is true with your social security number and other types of things. So, seven plus or minus two chunks of information is important. Now this seven plus or minus two has extreme implications for test taking. We demonstrate this in slide nine. Let’s say that you’re going to cram for an exam that you need to take in an hour. You’ve been studying all this stuff and you’ve been cramming to get the final pieces of information in there. Then you walk into the room and you get your test. What do you do? Well if you normally do things, you start with question one and answer it. Then you answer question two. By the time you get to question three, (which was the information that you were cramming for), lo and behold, the information is gone. Poof. So, what’s the solution to help yourself? Well, as soon as you get the exam, write the material down that you’re trying to remember as soon as you get it.

Here’s one example of the way people write things down. They take the first item and they write it down, they take the second item and write it down, then take the third item and write it down. Twenty seconds has now elapsed. So, when they get to the fourth item, what happens? Poof, it’s gone. So what’s the best way to record things? Well, write down the first item, then rehearse the list. That keeps the information in short term memory. Then you write down the second item and rehearse the list. And on and on until you have all the material that you’re tried to memorize. Then you can then use other techniques to answer the remaining questions.

So again, short term memory has a limited amount of material that you can store. It also has a limited duration of time which the information remains. So you have to be very careful with how you are working if you’re trying to retain stuff within a short term memory system.

Now, we have this information within short term memory and we’re trying to remember it. What happens to that information? Well ultimately it moves into the next stage as we saw in figure two, and that is called long term memory. Long term memory, as we see in slide 10, is where the information in essence goes and is retained.

Well, what’s the duration of long term memory? Well, it’s basically forever. The classic studies that really show memory duration was done by Walter Penfield the great Canadian neurosurgeon. What he did occurred while he was performing brain surgery on folks. During the procedure, he would use a small micro-electrode to stimulate parts of the brain while the patient was conscious. And this is a common technique that’s used in neurosurgery to identify structures and things that you do not want to destroy if at all possible. While the person is under surgery, Penfield would touch certain areas of the brain and memories would come back; memories that the person had not thought of for 10, 20, 30 or 40 years. For example, people in their 50s had childhood memories they had totally forgotten or repressed. So, it appears once we have the information in there, says Penfield and others, it seems to be retained forever.

So the question then becomes, “Hw do we get the information into long term memory?” We show this in slide 11. How we do this, says Atkinson and Shiffrin, is through rehearsal. We use information, rehearse the information in short term memory long enough for transfer to take place. So what’s this rehearsal thing called that you do? Atkinson and Shiffrin called it elaborative rehearsal. Basically (as we see in slide 12), what we do is relate information to information already stored in our long term memory. So we think of things, what they mean, and how they relate. Thus the better you organize the information, the easier it is to relate and store the information. So again, those storage factors and those organizational factors that we have discussed are very, very important to keeping and putting information into our long term memory system.
So now we’ve taken material in. We have got it into the system, we’ve got it through short term memory, and it’s now stuck in long term memory. The next question is, “How do you get it out?” This is shown in slide 13. There are a lot of different theories about how we get the information out, but two are the standards. The first one relates to retrieval cues and the second one relates to memory searches.

So let’s look at retrieval cues first on slide 14. Retrieval cues are basically stimuli that open pathways to the memory, and there are lots of different types of stimuli that you can use. For example, words, pictures, odors, sensations, etc. So cues, in essence, open some pathway and you then recall that information.

A related concept relates to what is called Encoding Specificity. In encoding specificity as we see in slide 15, the more specific the context where the item is learned to where the item is recalled, the higher the probability of the recall. That is, the more similar the context of the learning and recall conditions, the better the learning. So, it’s very similar to state dependent learning. So, finding the context (the item you are trying to recall) and how it is related to other items, the better the recall that you will have.

Now the next theory of how you get the information out relates to what we call memory searches. This is shown in slide 16. In general, what this theory contends is that people engage in memory searches for material; and we use one of two types. The first type is called a serial memory search. The second is a parallel memory search. So, let’s look at these in a little bit more detail.

Let’s look at a serial memory search first. A serial memory search is like a serial task. So, the first thing is look at the item and you ask, “Is it here?” If you say “No,” you then continue on. If it says yes, then you do something. So let’s say that you ask, “Is the item here?” If “Yes,” then you do something. If the system says, “No,” then the search looks in a different place and asks, “Is it here?” and if it says “Yes,” then you do something. If you say “No,” then you go on and you continue this type of searching pattern until ultimately you find the particular item. So in a serial memory search, what we have is a system where you go from one item or place to the next and then the next.
There is also a second type of memory search pattern and that is called a parallel search pattern. Again we discuss this on slide 16. In a parallel search, what you do is look for and find many items at once. So you have many searches going on inside the system. It is similar to how Microsoft Windows operates today (which is a much more powerful methodology than what we had several years ago). It is also much more powerful (and faster) than using a serial memory search.

So now we’ve talked about a variety of different things related to how we get information out. Well what kind of long term memories do we have? We begin on slide 18. There are a variety of different types of long term memory. These different types of memories are listed on slide 19.

As we can see here, we start with sensory memory. We then move the material into short term memory and then in the long term memory. Within long term memory we have a couple of different types, we have procedural memories and we have declarative memories which consist of systematic and episodic memories and so let’s talk about each of these.

Let’s talk about episodic memories first on slide 20. Basically an episodic memory is a memory for a very specific event. That is, what happened or where did it happen, etc. The classic example might be where you have your first kiss or where somebody asks you to marry them, or your first child, or whatever it may be. All of these things are very specific episodic memories.

The second type of long term memory system relates to what is called declarative memory and we see this on slide 21. These are memories for particular items of knowledge and they’re independent of the particular occasion in which they’re acquired. In essence, declarative memories are the sums of acquired knowledge that you have. All the works, all the symbols, all the facts of words, all the appearance of objects and on and on that we have within our memory system. This is the thing that makes you different from many other organisms that are out there. Not only do you remember these materials, you have been able and blessed with the capacity to pass on this material to your offspring, and offspring in generations far beyond your lifetime. So, the works that we have from Confucius and Buddha, and many other things, 4,000, 5,000 years BC, is very, very, important and can still be used and read today. So declarative memory is extremely important.

Now within declarative memories there are a couple of different types. The first of these, as we see in slide 22, is semantic memory. Semantic memory is basically the memory for words, the meanings of words and concepts. There are also lots of strategies we use for semantic memories. We use parallel searches, we use network models which like exploring things where we got go from one item to another to another to another.

The next method of memory is what we call visual memory and this is what we call eidetic imagery, or what most of you call photographic memory. Basically these are individuals who can recall detailed images of visual scenes for long periods of time. It’s extremely rare. Less than 5% of all kids are born with eidetic imagery, and less of that for adults. The reason for fewer adults is that it decreases as we get older and older. Also, kids rely on imagery more than adults.

The final memory that we talk about in long term memory are what are called procedural memories. These are memories for physical things like riding a bike, throwing a baseball, or whatever it may be.

So, in general, we have a variety of different memory systems which ultimately allows us to recall things for long periods of time.

In the next section we’re going to begin to discuss and examine some brain structures that are involved with memory.

Back